Tag

children

Browsing

Friends of the Nonverbal Communication  Blog, this week we present the paper “Causal indicators for assessing the truthfulness of child speech in forensic interviews”, by Durante, Z.; Ardulov, V.; Kumar, M.; Gongola, J.; Lyon, T. and Narayanan, S. (2022), in which authors carry out a study to identify any factor that is relevant to discern between true and false declarations when it’s about forensic interviews to children. 

In other articles we have seen how interviews are carried out to obtain testimony in controlled settings when it comes to legal proceedings and investigations involving children who may have been victims or witnesses of a crime.

The child is in a stage of human life in which he/she is especially vulnerable, but, in addition, he/she can be influenced more easily, even trained or forced to admit or omit false information.

That is, the same developmental attributes that make children more vulnerable also make their testimony susceptible to manipulation.

To address these issues, legal experts have developed a basic framework for properly conducting interviews, which should be carried out by professionals trained in the field.

The process begins with relationship building, where innocent open questions predominate to help put the child at ease. Afterwards, the interviewer moves on to a somewhat more critical part, during which she obtains memories, directing questions, also open, towards the topic of interest.

Because of all that is at stake, legal experts and psychologists are dedicated to finding factors that indicate whether a child is prepared to disclose information and whether the information disclosed by the child is true or false.

A meta-analysis of studies conducted a few years ago demonstrates the ability of adults to detect children’s lies, with an overall accuracy rate of 54%, which only increased to 59% when trained people were asked. These are not very high percentages.

This is thought to happen because adults tend to have a bias towards believing that a child’s statement is always true.

The hypothesis underlying the authors’ study is that the way children adapt their behavior in response to an interviewer’s behavior, is a more informative sign of deception than the behavior itself.

To combat the difficulties in discerning between truth and deceit in an interview with children, the protocols are administered by a trained professional to obtain reliable testimonies. These interviews are designed to minimize secondary victimization and maximize the retrieval of valuable information without coercion or leading questions.

When that first contact is established, the interviewer asks about innocuous topics so that the child feels comfortable talking; then there will be questions that relate directly to the investigation, without pressuring the child to reveal specific details.

Deception detection studies have been largely limited to adult subjects, using video, audio, or text. Previous work in this area with children is usually done on linguistic characteristics of the interview.

Rather, this article uses acoustic features and considers the child’s coordination and behavior in terms of the interviewer’s, to better understand the child’s dynamics and personality in the interview.

To do this, approximately 200 interviews were conducted, each one with a child, conducted by two experienced interviewers.

The session begins with the child and one of the interviewers, in a room full of toys. The interviewer begins to engage with the child, but one of the toys breaks and a transgression happens. This interviewer tells the child that a different interviewer will come in to ask him/her some questions, and adds that he/she should not say anything about the broken toy to avoid getting into trouble.

The second interviewer follows the basic protocol, first building a relationship of trust with the child and then talking to him/her about the toy so that he/she tells him what has happened to it.

It seems that the best predictor of whether or not a child is telling the truth is his/her level of imagination. There is a very important relationship between children’s and interviewers’ use of vivid language that evokes clear mental images.

Children who plan to omit that an occurred transgression choose their language more carefully, severely that of the interviewer. Therefore, the child becomes more or less vague in his/her descriptions, depending on the level of specificity the interviewer is using.

In contrast, if a boy/girl is honest, he/she will not modify his/her behavior based on the interviewer’s discourse.

This relationship suggests that interview protocols require interviewers to modulate imaginative levels in their language, in order to more reliably track and differentiate between true and false testimonies.

In the future, dynamic systems models that incorporate the interaction of the speaker and the child’s behavior may provide more information and improve accuracy.

If you want to know more about nonverbal behavior and how it influences our personal relationships, visit our Nonverbal Communication Certificate, a 100% online program certificated by the Heritage University (Washington) with special discounts for readers of the Nonverbal Communication Blog.


Mastering nonverbal behavior analysis is a fundamental skill in the professional world. With the Master in Nonverbal Behavior, you will not only learn to identify behavior patterns but also understand how they impact communication. This master’s program is 100% online, and upon completion, you will earn a degree from Evidentia University, a university licensed in the United States that guarantees international-quality education. Study from just $208 USD per month.

Friends of the Nonverbal Communication Blog, this week we present the paper “A comparison of Non-verbal Maternal Care of Male and Female Infants in India and the United Kingdom: The Parent-Infant Caregiving Touch Scale in Two Cultures”, by Hodsoll, J.; Pickles, A.; Bozicevic, L.; Supraja, T. A.; Hill, J.; Chandra, P. S. and Sharp, H. (2022), in which authors carry out two simultaneous studies, one in India and other in UK, to discover if there are any differences regarding how mothers behavior with their babies, focusing on “the loving touch”.

A mother’s care for her children is an essential characteristic for the correct development of mammals. Not only to guarantee their survival through their food and nutrition, but also for their physiological, cognitive, social and emotional development.

Care includes, in general terms, food, care for physical health, sensory and intellectual stimulation, security, emotional warmth and affection, comfort when in distress, and also the response to the needs and communications of the babies.

Although many of these aspects have been studied in depth, the role of touch has received very little attention and the results in this regard are much more limited.

In this paper, authors carry out a cross-cultural study to find out if there are differences in how Indian mothers and British mothers care for their children, specifically, when it comes to the so-called “loving touch”.

Why the “loving touch”? Because, in humans, touch-based care has many times been associated with benefits for babies. Skin-to-skin contact from early in the baby’s life in preterm infants has been associated with positive physiological outcomes for them.

It has also been shown to facilitate mother-infant interaction, making it more receptive and synchronous. It seems to support the development of emotional and cognitive skills in children, such as sustained attention or general control.

Specifically, early parental touch has a positive impact on the infant’s early behavior and physiological response to social stress, including her preschool emotional development.

Therefore, there is no doubt that touch is an important sensory exposure for babies that can shape their development.

As we already know, there are different traditions depending on the country in which we are and the behavior of care was not going to be less.

Several studies have examined early maternal sensitivity in certain cultures, and there is some debate, with some experts arguing that there is no universality of maternal sensitivity.

On the other hand, there are other experts who do support the universality of maternal sensitivity and affirm that its manifestations vary between cultures.

The truth is that care responses can be found from mother to child in all cultures, but they are different. For example, cross-national studies have shown that when Western mothers interact with their babies, they use more proximal behaviors: caressing or patting, for example. They also use distant behaviors, such as talking, looking, or smiling. But in other non-Western cultures, such as India, the baby is massaged daily.

On the other hand, authors were interested in exploring whether there was any variation in the care received by the baby depending on whether he/she was a boy or a girl. In some South Asian populations, boys are favored over girls, and this preference might be relevant to the mothers themselves. In fact, India has the most abnormal levels of female excess mortality in the world in girls aged 1-4 years.

Authors hypothesized that boys would receive greater tactile attention compared to girls.

In the Indian context, a total of 395 mothers participated, and in the United Kingdom, 874. The Parent-Infant Caregiving Touch Scale (PICTS) was produced, which contains questions such as: how often does the mother touch her baby’s back, head , your tummy, arms or legs.

The findings were clear: the rate of reported early caresses in the UK is higher than in India, but there was no difference regarding the sex of the babies.

That is to say, on the one hand, it seems that cultural differences are significant and in India it is much more common to practice daily baby massages than caress, unlike in the United Kingdom. On the other hand, it seems that sexual discrimination is experiencing a certain decrease.

Authors mention that this article may serve to examine in the future if the protective effects that are supposed to appear after these care behaviors are similar for children in India and the United Kingdom, seeing that the care practices are different.

If you want to know more about nonverbal behavior and how it influences our personal relationships, visit our Nonverbal Communication Certificate, a 100% online program certificated by the Heritage University (Washington) with special discounts for readers of the Nonverbal Communication Blog.


Mastering nonverbal behavior analysis is a fundamental skill in the professional world. With the Master in Nonverbal Behavior, you will not only learn to identify behavior patterns but also understand how they impact communication. This master’s program is 100% online, and upon completion, you will earn a degree from Evidentia University, a university licensed in the United States that guarantees international-quality education. Study from just $208 USD per month.

NonVerbal Communication Blog